NVESTIGATING LEXICAL ACQUISITION PATTERNS: CONTEXT AND COGNITION



Coyle (1984) compared words in context to words in isolation. Children aged 8 and 10
years were asked first to choose the correct meaning (from four alternatives) of unfamiliar
words in isolation. The words were then presented in passages constructed to provide
contextual cues. The children were told to read the passages, either with a neutral
instruction, or an instruction alerting them to contextual information. The word meanings
had to be chosen again from four possibilities. Scores on the words-in-context were
significantly higher than on the words-in isolation test, but there was no effect of calling
attention to the contextual cues by instruction. Children use context to infer meaning of
novel words without being directly instructed to do so.

The studies discussed so far indicate that some learning from context does occur.
However, a most interesting facet of the literature in this area is that the power of
contextual effects is interpreted in markedly different ways by different researchers. Nagy
et. al. (1985) characterize the effect as substantial and hypothesize that the overall
influence of context on vocabulary learning is large because the volume of reading
students typically do, allows for a great accumulation of encounters with unknown words,
and ultimately, learning of a substantial numbers of words. On the other hand, Jenkins,
Stein and Wysoski (1984) conclude that learning from context does not come easily or in
large quantities.

2.4.2 Decoding of Internal Context

By internal context, Sternberg and Powell (1983) refer to the morphemes within a word.
Multiple morphemes such as prefixes, suffixes, and stems, give the word meaning. Again,
the theory proposes a set of contextual cues and a set of mediating variables that together
determine the quality of a definition that can be inferred from a word’s internal context.

Contextual Cues

Four types of contextual cues have been identified: Prefix cues; Stem cues; Suffix cues;
Interactive cues. Interactive cues are formed when two or even three of the word parts
listed above convey information in combination that is not conveyed by a given cue
considered in isolation from the rest of the word. The usefulness of these kinds of cues in
decoding meaning can be shown by the following example used by Stemberg and Powell
(1983).

63



More intriguing information

1. The name is absent
2. What should educational research do, and how should it do it? A response to “Will a clinical approach make educational research more relevant to practice” by Jacquelien Bulterman-Bos
3. How we might be able to understand the brain
4. Heavy Hero or Digital Dummy: multimodal player-avatar relations in FINAL FANTASY 7
5. EMU: some unanswered questions
6. Deprivation Analysis in Declining Inner City Residential Areas: A Case Study From Izmir, Turkey.
7. The Impact of Optimal Tariffs and Taxes on Agglomeration
8. Evolutionary Clustering in Indonesian Ethnic Textile Motifs
9. The name is absent
10. Visual Perception of Humanoid Movement
11. The name is absent
12. Ventas callejeras y espacio público: efectos sobre el comercio de Bogotá
13. A COMPARATIVE STUDY OF ALTERNATIVE ECONOMETRIC PACKAGES: AN APPLICATION TO ITALIAN DEPOSIT INTEREST RATES
14. The name is absent
15. 101 Proposals to reform the Stability and Growth Pact. Why so many? A Survey
16. NVESTIGATING LEXICAL ACQUISITION PATTERNS: CONTEXT AND COGNITION
17. A novel selective 11b-hydroxysteroid dehydrogenase type 1 inhibitor prevents human adipogenesis
18. Does Presenting Patients’ BMI Increase Documentation of Obesity?
19. Benchmarking Regional Innovation: A Comparison of Bavaria, Northern Ireland and the Republic of Ireland
20. Experimental Evidence of Risk Aversion in Consumer Markets: The Case of Beef Tenderness