NVESTIGATING LEXICAL ACQUISITION PATTERNS: CONTEXT AND COGNITION



mauve; it’s a kind of purple”). The children were tested on their interpretation of the
novel word immediately following its introduction. The measurements used were a
generalization task, co-hyponym task, colour identification task and relations task.

Four- and five-year-old children interpreted the novel word as a shape term when
ostensive information was provided but as a colour term when additional information,
either contrastive or inclusive, specified a relation between the novel term and a known
label for that colour. Additionally, children who consistently interpreted the novel word
as a colour word tended to treat the novel and known labels as mutually exclusive colour
terms if they heard contrastive information, whereas they tended to treat the words as
hierarchically related if they heard inclusion information. Three-year-olds generally did
not make use of either type of information in determining the semantic domain of the
novel word or the relation between the terms.

The importance of input for word learning was shown in all the previous studies.
Nevertheless, the previous studies are characterised by an artificial type of exposure to
the novel words (e.g, isolated sentences, single assessments). In the next section, a review
of studies using a different mode of presentation of the input, such as stories, follows.

3.2.2 Evidence from listening to stories

Reading stories to preschool children has been recognised, by many researchers, as
beneficial to the development of their literacy skills (Clark 1984; Wells, 1986). The
longitudinal data from Wells’ study (1986) showed that listening to stories at preschool
age was positively related to teachers’ assessments of vocabulary size at age ten. A lot of
research has also concentrated on primary school children, and whether they can acquire
specific new vocabulary terms from reading. Only a handful of researchers have looked
at whether preschool age children can acquire new lexical items from listening to story
readings (Eller, Pappas & Brown, 1988; Elley 1989; Robbins & Ehri, 1994; Senechal &
Cornell, 1993; Leung & Pikulski, 1990).

Eller, Pappas & Brown (1988) designed a study to explore the process of incidental
vocabulary learning. Two illustrated storybooks were used, written by Brian Wildsmith.
These were “The Owl and the Woodpecker” and “The Lazy Bear”. Twenty target lexical

71



More intriguing information

1. A Rational Analysis of Alternating Search and Reflection Strategies in Problem Solving
2. FDI Implications of Recent European Court of Justice Decision on Corporation Tax Matters
3. A simple enquiry on heterogeneous lending rates and lending behaviour
4. The Impact of Cognitive versus Affective Aspects on Consumer Usage of Financial Service Delivery Channels
5. Competition In or For the Field: Which is Better
6. The Dictator and the Parties A Study on Policy Co-operation in Mineral Economies
7. Policy Formulation, Implementation and Feedback in EU Merger Control
8. A production model and maintenance planning model for the process industry
9. Methods for the thematic synthesis of qualitative research in systematic reviews
10. Cancer-related electronic support groups as navigation-aids: Overcoming geographic barriers
11. A Bayesian approach to analyze regional elasticities
12. The name is absent
13. The name is absent
14. ISSUES AND PROBLEMS OF IMMEDIATE CONCERN
15. An Efficient Circulant MIMO Equalizer for CDMA Downlink: Algorithm and VLSI Architecture
16. Should Local Public Employment Services be Merged with the Local Social Benefit Administrations?
17. Innovation Trajectories in Honduras’ Coffee Value Chain. Public and Private Influence on the Use of New Knowledge and Technology among Coffee Growers
18. The name is absent
19. The name is absent
20. An alternative way to model merit good arguments