NVESTIGATING LEXICAL ACQUISITION PATTERNS: CONTEXT AND COGNITION



students could produce any correct associations to a word); and semantic depth (i.e.,
whether they could produce a synonym or correct explanation). Curtis found that low-
ability students not only knew fewer of the tested words and had less practice knowledge
of the words they knew, they were also able to produce correct explanations for only
about half the words they had correct on the multiple choice test. Broader knowledge of
words is seen on tasks assessing antonyms, synonyms, hyponyms, semantic attributes.

Antonyms require the student either to use learned word associations (e.g., hot-cold) or
to analyse the characteristics of the stimulus and to choose which characteristic is the
critical one to oppose. Jorgensen et. al. (1981) suggest that failure to produce antonyms
may reflect (a) inability to determine the steps necessary for antonymous responses, (b)
difficulty in focusing on the critical semantic dimension, (c ) vocabulary that does not
include an opposite lexical item for each stimulus, (d) inability to retrieve the opposite
term, or (e) difficulty in understanding the meaning of the stimulus.

A synonym task can also be performed with learned responses (purse=handbag) or by
completing the analysis steps to find sufficient similarities of attributes. A related task is
that of similarities or differences, where the student is asked to choose which items are
either similar or different among three or four items and then may be asked to give a
reason for their choice. Like the antonym or synonym tasks, the student must have
available the appropriate semantic categories, knowledge of membership attributes, and
ability to compare∕contrast the items by their attributes.

The hyponym is another task that assess whether the child has some knowledge of the
domain to which the new word belongs (e.g, does the child knows that beige is a colour
word ?) and can make proper contrasts within the domain. In Carey and Bartlett’s (1978)
hyponym task, children were asked if various words were colours. Questions were of the
following type: “Is purple a colour ?” “Is cold a colour ?” “Is noisy a colour ?” A child had
to answer all of the questions correctly in order to be credited with knowing that
chromium is a colour word. This task proved to be very difficult for the children.

Heibeck and Markman (1987) used also the hyponym measure by asking the children to
provide a proper contrast for the new word. For example, children might hear: “See this?

83



More intriguing information

1. The Response of Ethiopian Grain Markets to Liberalization
2. The name is absent
3. REVITALIZING FAMILY FARM AGRICULTURE
4. Computing optimal sampling designs for two-stage studies
5. Business Networks and Performance: A Spatial Approach
6. The name is absent
7. Visual Artists Between Cultural Demand and Economic Subsistence. Empirical Findings From Berlin.
8. The name is absent
9. Concerns for Equity and the Optimal Co-Payments for Publicly Provided Health Care
10. Dual Inflation Under the Currency Board: The Challenges of Bulgarian EU Accession
11. Wirkung einer Feiertagsbereinigung des Länderfinanzausgleichs: eine empirische Analyse des deutschen Finanzausgleichs
12. Learning-by-Exporting? Firm-Level Evidence for UK Manufacturing and Services Sectors
13. A Regional Core, Adjacent, Periphery Model for National Economic Geography Analysis
14. An Incentive System for Salmonella Control in the Pork Supply Chain
15. Impacts of Tourism and Fiscal Expenditure on Remote Islands in Japan: A Panel Data Analysis
16. Improving the Impact of Market Reform on Agricultural Productivity in Africa: How Institutional Design Makes a Difference
17. Optimal Rent Extraction in Pre-Industrial England and France – Default Risk and Monitoring Costs
18. The name is absent
19. Estimating the Impact of Medication on Diabetics' Diet and Lifestyle Choices
20. Innovation in commercialization of pelagic fish: the example of "Srdela Snack" Franchise