Feature type effects in semantic memory: An event related potentials study



144


G. Sartori et al. / Neuroscience Letters 390 (2005) 139-144

shows up in larger N400, then larger N400 are expected for
these two types of items if relevance is not controlled. In
contrast, when relevance is matched, any category or fea-
ture type effect should vanish; and this is what we found. At
neural level, we showed that relevance matched categories
had similar N400 and that relevance matched feature types
had also similar N400. In other words, no difference between
Living and Non-living and between Sensory and Non-sensory
descriptions may be found when relevance is matched. These
results confirmed the view that the previously reported dis-
sociations observed using ERPs could be spurious.

In our view, the larger N400 for Living [11] and for Sen-
sory features
[3] may not be genuine effects if we consider
that: (i) low relevance semantic features elicit larger N400 and
(ii) Living items have, on average, many Sensory features of
lower relevance as compared to Non-living
[19]. Therefore,
any uncontrolled set of stimuli is likely to result in larger,
spurious N400 for items belonging to the Living category
and in larger N400 for Sensory descriptions.

These results increase credibility to the general claim that
the organising principles of conceptual representation in the
brain are semantic features rather than categories (see also
[17]). Aside from a clear relevance effect, also the absence of
any category effect is in accord with this view. With regard
to semantic features, we present results at neural level that
parallel those previously reported at behavioural level
[19].
Taken together our results seem to indicate that semantic
features may not be organised on the basis of their content
(Sensory versus Non-sensory) but rather on the basis of their
importance in facilitating concept retrieval. Relevance, an
effective index of this importance, may account for many
effects previously believed to characterise the organisation at
neural level. Instead, feature content per se does not affect
ERPs. Our data raise the possibility that it is the impor-
tance of semantic features (relevance), which is the basis
of behavioural and neural effects of category and feature
types that were previously reported. In sum, this investigation
adds credibility to the sceptic views on category-specificity
as researchers are looking more closely at criteria used in
defining the phenomenon. In fact, credibility of semantic
memory dissociations, at behavioural level, is reduced by a
number of methodological problems concerning the methods
through which dissociations are established
[4]. Experimen-
tal approaches to the neural basis of semantic memory are
just beginning to control the effect of such variables on pro-
cessing requirements after a period in which the only variable
manipulated was category (see
[9]).

References

[1] S.F. Cappa, D. Perani, T. Schnur, M. Tettamanti, F. Fazio, The effects
of semantic category and knowledge type on lexical-semantic access:
a PET study, Neuroimage 8 (1998) 350-359.

[2] A. Caramazza, J.R. Shelton, Domain-specific knowledge systems in
the brain, J. Cogn. Neurosci. 10 (1998) 1-34.

[3] M. Coltheart, L. Inglis, L. Cupples, P. Michie, A. Bates, B. Budd,
A semantic subsystem of visual attributes, Neurocase 4 (1998)
353-370.

[4] G.S. Cree, K. McRae, Analyzing the factors underlying the structure
and computation of the meaning of chipmunk, cherry, chisel, and
cello (and many other such concrete nouns), J. Exp. Psychol.: Gen.
132 (2003) 163-201.

[5] R. Dell’Acqua, L. Lotto, R. Job, Naming time and standard-
ized norms for the Italian PD/DPSS of 266 pictures: direct com-
parisons with American, English, French, and Spanish published
databases, Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 32 (2000) 588-
612.

[6] J.T. Devlin, C.J. Moore, C.J. Mummery, M.L. Gorno-Tempini, J.A.
Phillips, U. Noppeney, R.S.J. Frackowiak, K.J. Friston, C.J. Price,
Anatomic constraints on cognitive theories of category specificity,
Neuroimage 15 (2002) 675-685.

[7] M.J. Farah, J.L. McClelland, A computational model of seman-
tic memory impairment: modality specificity and emergent category
specificity, J. Exp. Psychol.: Gen. 120 (1991) 339-357.

[8] K.D. Federmeier, M. Kutas, A rose by any other name: long-term
memory structure and sentence processing, J. Mem. Lang. 41 (1999)
469-495.

[9] J. Joseph, Functional neuroimaging studies of category specificity in
object recognition: a critical review and meta-analysis, Cogn. Affect.
Behav. Neurosci. 1 (2001) 119-136.

[10] M. Kiang, M. Kutas, Association of schizotypy with seman-
tic processing differences: an event-related brain potentials study,
Schizophr. Res. (2005) 329-342.

[11] M. Kiefer, Perceptual and semantic source of category-specific
effects: event-related potentials during picture and word categoriza-
tion, Mem. Cogn. 29 (2001) 100-116.

[12] M. Kutas, K.D. Federmeier, Electrophysiology reveals semantic
memory use in language comprehension, Trends Cogn. Sci. 4 (2000)
463-470.

[13] M. Kutas, S.A. Hillyard, Reading senseless sentences: brain poten-
tials reflect semantic incongruity, Science 297 (1980) 203-205.

[14] A. Martin, L.L. Chao, Semantic memory and the brain: structure and
processes, Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 11 (2001) 194-201.

[15] A. Martin, C.L. Wiggs, L.G. Ungerleider, J.V. Haxby, Neural cor-
relates of category-specific knowledge, Nature 379 (1996) 649-
652.

[16] R. McCarthy, E.K. Warrington, Category specificity in an agram-
matic patient: the relative impairment of verb retrieval and compre-
hension, Neuropsychologia 23 (1985) 709-727.

[17] C.J. Mummery, T. Shallice, C.J. Price, Dual-process model in seman-
tic priming: a functional imaging perspective, Neuroimage 9 (1999)
516-525.

[18] E. Saffran, The organization of semantic memory: in support of a
distributed model, Brain Lang. 71 (2000) 204-212.

[19] G. Sartori, L. Lombardi, Semantic relevance and semantic disorders,
J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16 (2004) 439-452.

[20] G. Sartori, L. Lombardi, L. Mattiuzzi, Semantic relevance best pre-
dicts normal and abnormal name retrieval, Neuropsychologia (2005)
754-770.

[21] S.L. Thompson-Schill, G.K. Aguirre, M. Desposito, M.J. Farah, A
neural basis for category and modality specificity of semantic knowl-
edge, Neuropsychologia 37 (1999) 671-676.

[22] E.K. Warrington, T. Shallice, Category specific semantic impair-
ments, Brain 107 (1999) 829-854.

[23] T. Yano, M. Kaga, Semantic category discrimination and N400, Psy-
chol. Rep. 87 (2000) 415-422.



More intriguing information

1. Categorial Grammar and Discourse
2. Correlates of Alcoholic Blackout Experience
3. Target Acquisition in Multiscale Electronic Worlds
4. The name is absent
5. Manufacturing Earnings and Cycles: New Evidence
6. ANTI-COMPETITIVE FINANCIAL CONTRACTING: THE DESIGN OF FINANCIAL CLAIMS.
7. The name is absent
8. The name is absent
9. Ronald Patterson, Violinist; Brooks Smith, Pianist
10. THE ECONOMICS OF COMPETITION IN HEALTH INSURANCE- THE IRISH CASE STUDY.
11. Foreign Direct Investment and Unequal Regional Economic Growth in China
12. Existentialism: a Philosophy of Hope or Despair?
13. The Impact of EU Accession in Romania: An Analysis of Regional Development Policy Effects by a Multiregional I-O Model
14. Ventas callejeras y espacio público: efectos sobre el comercio de Bogotá
15. The open method of co-ordination: Some remarks regarding old-age security within an enlarged European Union
16. Demographic Features, Beliefs And Socio-Psychological Impact Of Acne Vulgaris Among Its Sufferers In Two Towns In Nigeria
17. Evaluating Consumer Usage of Nutritional Labeling: The Influence of Socio-Economic Characteristics
18. Tariff Escalation and Invasive Species Risk
19. Parallel and overlapping Human Immunodeficiency Virus, Hepatitis B and C virus Infections among pregnant women in the Federal Capital Territory, Abuja, Nigeria
20. The name is absent