[20] Markus, H., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for
cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224-253.
[21] Masuda, T., & Nisbett, R.E. (2001). Attending holistically versus ana-
lytically: Comparing the context sensitivity of Japanese and Americans.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 922-934.
[22] McClure, S.M., Laibson, D.I., Loewenstein, G., & Cohen, J.D. (2004).
Separate neural systems value immediate and delayed monetary rewards.
Science, 306 (5695), 503-507.
[23] McClure, S.M., Ericson, K.M., Laibson, D.I., Loewenstein, G., & Cohen,
J.D. (2007). Time discounting for primary rewards. Journal of Neuro-
science, 27 (21), 5796-5804.
[24] Michael, F., & Johnson, M.D. (2003). Financial market dynamics. Physica
A: Statistical Mechanics and its Applications, 320, 525-534.
[25] Monterosso, J.R., Ainslie, G., Xu, J., Cordova, X., Domier, C.P., & Lon-
don, E.D. (2007). Frontoparietal cortical activity of methamphetamine-
dependent and comparison subjects performing a delay discounting task.
Human Brain Mapping, 28 (5), 383-393.
[26] Read, D., & Roelofsma, P.H.M.P. (2003). Subadditive versus hyperbolic
discounting: A comparison of choice and matching. Organizational Be-
havior and Human Decision Processes, 91 (2), 140-153.
[27] Sanfey, A.G., Loewenstein, G., McClure, S.M., & Cohen, J.D. (2006).
Neuroeconomics: Cross-currents in research on decision-making. Trends
in Cognitive Sciences, 10 (3), 108-116.
[28] Soman, D., Ainslie, G., Frederick, S., Li, X., Lynch, J., Moreau, P.,
Mitchell, A., Read, D., Sawyer, A., Trope, Y., Wertenbroch, K., & Zauber-
man, G. (2005). The psychology of intertemporal discounting: Why are
distant events valued differently from proximal ones? Marketing Letters,
16 (3-4), 347-360.
[29] Stariolo, D. A. (1994). The Langevin and Fokker-Planck equations in the
framework of a generalized statistical mechanics. Physics Letters A, 185,
262-264.
[30] Takahashi, T. (2005). Loss of self-control in intertemporal choice may be
attributable to logarithmic time-perception. Medical Hypotheses, 65 (4),
691-693.
[31] Takahashi, T. (2006). Time-estimation error following Weber-Fechner law
may explain subadditive time-discounting. Medical Hypotheses, 67 (6),
1372-1374.
[32] Takahashi, T., Ikeda, K., & Hasegawa, T. (2007). A hyperbolic decay of
subjective probability of obtaining delayed rewards. Behavioral and Brain
Functions, 3, 52.
More intriguing information
1. TOWARDS THE ZERO ACCIDENT GOAL: ASSISTING THE FIRST OFFICER MONITOR AND CHALLENGE CAPTAIN ERRORS2. From music student to professional: the process of transition
3. The name is absent
4. Growth and Technological Leadership in US Industries: A Spatial Econometric Analysis at the State Level, 1963-1997
5. Public-private sector pay differentials in a devolved Scotland
6. Moffett and rhetoric
7. Strategic Effects and Incentives in Multi-issue Bargaining Games
8. The name is absent
9. Impact of Ethanol Production on U.S. and Regional Gasoline Prices and On the Profitability of U.S. Oil Refinery Industry
10. What should educational research do, and how should it do it? A response to “Will a clinical approach make educational research more relevant to practice” by Jacquelien Bulterman-Bos