Cultural Neuroeconomics of Intertemporal Choice



[20] Markus, H., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for
cognition, emotion, and motivation.
Psychological Review, 98, 224-253.

[21] Masuda, T., & Nisbett, R.E. (2001). Attending holistically versus ana-
lytically: Comparing the context sensitivity of Japanese and Americans.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 922-934.

[22] McClure, S.M., Laibson, D.I., Loewenstein, G., & Cohen, J.D. (2004).
Separate neural systems value immediate and delayed monetary rewards.
Science, 306 (5695), 503-507.

[23] McClure, S.M., Ericson, K.M., Laibson, D.I., Loewenstein, G., & Cohen,
J.D. (2007). Time discounting for primary rewards.
Journal of Neuro-
science
, 27 (21), 5796-5804.

[24] Michael, F., & Johnson, M.D. (2003). Financial market dynamics. Physica
A: Statistical Mechanics and its Applications
, 320, 525-534.

[25] Monterosso, J.R., Ainslie, G., Xu, J., Cordova, X., Domier, C.P., & Lon-
don, E.D. (2007). Frontoparietal cortical activity of methamphetamine-
dependent and comparison subjects performing a delay discounting task.
Human Brain Mapping, 28 (5), 383-393.

[26] Read, D., & Roelofsma, P.H.M.P. (2003). Subadditive versus hyperbolic
discounting: A comparison of choice and matching.
Organizational Be-
havior and Human Decision Processes
, 91 (2), 140-153.

[27] Sanfey, A.G., Loewenstein, G., McClure, S.M., & Cohen, J.D. (2006).
Neuroeconomics: Cross-currents in research on decision-making.
Trends
in Cognitive Sciences
, 10 (3), 108-116.

[28] Soman, D., Ainslie, G., Frederick, S., Li, X., Lynch, J., Moreau, P.,
Mitchell, A., Read, D., Sawyer, A., Trope, Y., Wertenbroch, K., & Zauber-
man, G. (2005). The psychology of intertemporal discounting: Why are
distant events valued differently from proximal ones?
Marketing Letters,
16 (3-4), 347-360.

[29] Stariolo, D. A. (1994). The Langevin and Fokker-Planck equations in the
framework of a generalized statistical mechanics.
Physics Letters A, 185,
262-264.

[30] Takahashi, T. (2005). Loss of self-control in intertemporal choice may be
attributable to logarithmic time-perception.
Medical Hypotheses, 65 (4),
691-693.

[31] Takahashi, T. (2006). Time-estimation error following Weber-Fechner law
may explain subadditive time-discounting.
Medical Hypotheses, 67 (6),
1372-1374.

[32] Takahashi, T., Ikeda, K., & Hasegawa, T. (2007). A hyperbolic decay of
subjective probability of obtaining delayed rewards.
Behavioral and Brain
Functions
, 3, 52.



More intriguing information

1. El Mercosur y la integración económica global
2. The name is absent
3. Internationalization of Universities as Internationalization of Bildung
4. Putting Globalization and Concentration in the Agri-food Sector into Context
5. The name is absent
6. Can we design a market for competitive health insurance? CHERE Discussion Paper No 53
7. Can genetic algorithms explain experimental anomalies? An application to common property resources
8. SME'S SUPPORT AND REGIONAL POLICY IN EU - THE NORTE-LITORAL PORTUGUESE EXPERIENCE
9. The name is absent
10. IMPLICATIONS OF CHANGING AID PROGRAMS TO U.S. AGRICULTURE
11. A Rare Presentation of Crohn's Disease
12. The name is absent
13. Wirkt eine Preisregulierung nur auf den Preis?: Anmerkungen zu den Wirkungen einer Preisregulierung auf das Werbevolumen
14. An Incentive System for Salmonella Control in the Pork Supply Chain
15. The Response of Ethiopian Grain Markets to Liberalization
16. The name is absent
17. Incorporating global skills within UK higher education of engineers
18. The name is absent
19. PROFITABILITY OF ALFALFA HAY STORAGE USING PROBABILITIES: AN EXTENSION APPROACH
20. A parametric approach to the estimation of cointegration vectors in panel data