Cultural Neuroeconomics of Intertemporal Choice



[20] Markus, H., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for
cognition, emotion, and motivation.
Psychological Review, 98, 224-253.

[21] Masuda, T., & Nisbett, R.E. (2001). Attending holistically versus ana-
lytically: Comparing the context sensitivity of Japanese and Americans.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 922-934.

[22] McClure, S.M., Laibson, D.I., Loewenstein, G., & Cohen, J.D. (2004).
Separate neural systems value immediate and delayed monetary rewards.
Science, 306 (5695), 503-507.

[23] McClure, S.M., Ericson, K.M., Laibson, D.I., Loewenstein, G., & Cohen,
J.D. (2007). Time discounting for primary rewards.
Journal of Neuro-
science
, 27 (21), 5796-5804.

[24] Michael, F., & Johnson, M.D. (2003). Financial market dynamics. Physica
A: Statistical Mechanics and its Applications
, 320, 525-534.

[25] Monterosso, J.R., Ainslie, G., Xu, J., Cordova, X., Domier, C.P., & Lon-
don, E.D. (2007). Frontoparietal cortical activity of methamphetamine-
dependent and comparison subjects performing a delay discounting task.
Human Brain Mapping, 28 (5), 383-393.

[26] Read, D., & Roelofsma, P.H.M.P. (2003). Subadditive versus hyperbolic
discounting: A comparison of choice and matching.
Organizational Be-
havior and Human Decision Processes
, 91 (2), 140-153.

[27] Sanfey, A.G., Loewenstein, G., McClure, S.M., & Cohen, J.D. (2006).
Neuroeconomics: Cross-currents in research on decision-making.
Trends
in Cognitive Sciences
, 10 (3), 108-116.

[28] Soman, D., Ainslie, G., Frederick, S., Li, X., Lynch, J., Moreau, P.,
Mitchell, A., Read, D., Sawyer, A., Trope, Y., Wertenbroch, K., & Zauber-
man, G. (2005). The psychology of intertemporal discounting: Why are
distant events valued differently from proximal ones?
Marketing Letters,
16 (3-4), 347-360.

[29] Stariolo, D. A. (1994). The Langevin and Fokker-Planck equations in the
framework of a generalized statistical mechanics.
Physics Letters A, 185,
262-264.

[30] Takahashi, T. (2005). Loss of self-control in intertemporal choice may be
attributable to logarithmic time-perception.
Medical Hypotheses, 65 (4),
691-693.

[31] Takahashi, T. (2006). Time-estimation error following Weber-Fechner law
may explain subadditive time-discounting.
Medical Hypotheses, 67 (6),
1372-1374.

[32] Takahashi, T., Ikeda, K., & Hasegawa, T. (2007). A hyperbolic decay of
subjective probability of obtaining delayed rewards.
Behavioral and Brain
Functions
, 3, 52.



More intriguing information

1. The Impact of Minimum Wages on Wage Inequality and Employment in the Formal and Informal Sector in Costa Rica
2. El Mercosur y la integración económica global
3. IMPROVING THE UNIVERSITY'S PERFORMANCE IN PUBLIC POLICY EDUCATION
4. Road pricing and (re)location decisions households
5. A Bayesian approach to analyze regional elasticities
6. Portuguese Women in Science and Technology (S&T): Some Gender Features Behind MSc. and PhD. Achievement
7. The name is absent
8. The name is absent
9. The name is absent
10. The name is absent