The effect of classroom diversity on tolerance and participation in England, Sweden and Germany



refutation of the conflict hypothesis (remember that that latter expects tolerance levels
among dominant group members to fall as the proportion of minorities increases).
As
noted earlier, since the items composing the ethnic tolerance index all refer to immigrants
I believe that the index essentially captures the tolerance levels of the ethnic majority
only. To then find that ethnic majority respondents are indeed more tolerant the more
diverse their classroom is all the more supports the contact argument.

Table 3 about here

In England, however, the relation between heterogeneity and tolerance changes from a
positive to a not-significant negative one for all respondents (Model II) and for the ethnic
majority (Model III) once individual factors are included in the analysis. A possible
explanation for the different results of the white British students compared to the ethnic
German and Swedish students is the longer history of immigration in Britain. It could
well be that this longer history has made white British youth, also those in mono-ethnic
schools, become more accustomed to ethnic minorities than their counterparts in Sweden
and Germany. In other words, the longer presence of ethnic minorities in Britain may
have had the effect of leveling out attitudinal differences between students in diverse and
homogenous classes. If this is indeed the causal mechanism then we should expect to see
the diversity effect dissipate in Sweden and Germany as well with the passing of time.
Unfortunately, this hypothesis cannot be tested with the data at hand.

Another possibility is that the political tradition of liberalism and multiculturalism
in England imposes such strong norms of tolerance that the ethnic composition of
classrooms essentially has little to add. In other words, white British students in all-white
classrooms feel the same pressure to give socially desirable responses on the tolerance
items as white British students in mixed classrooms. If this conjecture is true one would
expect the overall levels of tolerance to be higher in England than in the other countries.
The descriptive statistics of Table 1, however, show that the tolerance levels of white
British students are
below the international mean of ten while those of their Swedish
counterparts are above this mean. Thus this hypothesis is unlikely to have much

16



More intriguing information

1. The name is absent
2. A Review of Kuhnian and Lakatosian “Explanations” in Economics
3. The name is absent
4. TWENTY-FIVE YEARS OF RESEARCH ON WOMEN FARMERS IN AFRICA: LESSONS AND IMPLICATIONS FOR AGRICULTURAL RESEARCH INSTITUTIONS; WITH AN ANNOTATED BIBLIOGRAPHY
5. The urban sprawl dynamics: does a neural network understand the spatial logic better than a cellular automata?
6. Constrained School Choice
7. fMRI Investigation of Cortical and Subcortical Networks in the Learning of Abstract and Effector-Specific Representations of Motor Sequences
8. Financial Development and Sectoral Output Growth in 19th Century Germany
9. Individual tradable permit market and traffic congestion: An experimental study
10. Inflation Targeting and Nonlinear Policy Rules: The Case of Asymmetric Preferences (new title: The Fed's monetary policy rule and U.S. inflation: The case of asymmetric preferences)
11. The name is absent
12. Categorial Grammar and Discourse
13. Cultural Neuroeconomics of Intertemporal Choice
14. The name is absent
15. Foreign direct investment in the Indian telecommunications sector
16. Gender stereotyping and wage discrimination among Italian graduates
17. The name is absent
18. Correlates of Alcoholic Blackout Experience
19. Declining Discount Rates: Evidence from the UK
20. Non Linear Contracting and Endogenous Buyer Power between Manufacturers and Retailers: Empirical Evidence on Food Retailing in France