93
alone could not reliably produce a somatosensory percept. However, there was a
significant interaction between the auditory stimulus and somatosensory stimulus
factors (F1,ιg = 6.69, p = .02), showing that sound can modulate somatosensory
perception. As shown in Figure 1 and Table 1, this interaction was due to a significant
increase in the detection rate for somatosensory stimuli when they were accompanied
by an auditory stimulus (61.6% vs. 57.4%; tιg = 2.121, p = .047). Importantly, although
the sound increased the detection rate when a somatosensory stimulus was presented,
the sound did not increase the false alarm rate for reporting a somatosensory stimulus
when none was presented (3.4% for sound present vs. 3.4% for sound absent; F < 1).
This shows that increase in the detection rate for somatosensory stimuli with sounds
was not due to confounding factors, such as feeling mechanical vibrations or air
pressure from the speakers.

Figure 1. The data from Experiment 1 examining the effects of audition on touch perception.
The left half of the figure shows the hit rates, whereas the right half of the figure illustrates
the false alarm rates. Error bars reflect ±1 standard error of the mean.
More intriguing information
1. Fertility in Developing Countries2. Wage mobility, Job mobility and Spatial mobility in the Portuguese economy
3. The name is absent
4. The name is absent
5. PROFITABILITY OF ALFALFA HAY STORAGE USING PROBABILITIES: AN EXTENSION APPROACH
6. The name is absent
7. Consumer Networks and Firm Reputation: A First Experimental Investigation
8. NATURAL RESOURCE SUPPLY CONSTRAINTS AND REGIONAL ECONOMIC ANALYSIS: A COMPUTABLE GENERAL EQUILIBRIUM APPROACH
9. Sectoral specialisation in the EU a macroeconomic perspective
10. Migration and employment status during the turbulent nineties in Sweden