would allow the child to switch from one language to another without 'confusion,
without translation, without a mother tongue accent'.
The concept of a critical period for language learning was further developed by
Lenneberg (1967) in his 'critical period hypothesis' (CPH). In his studies on
cerebral injuries and the Iateralisation of brain functions he found that unlike
children, adults could not transfer language functions between the two brain
hemispheres. Lenneberg linked the completion of brain Iateralisation to the
onset of puberty which marked the end of the 'critical period' for language
learning. Like Penfield's work, Lenneberg1S work was also carried out in the
context offirst language acquisition and like Penfield, he subsequently claimed
a 'biological clock' and a critical period for the learning of a second language:
"... automatic acquisition from mere exposure to a given language seems to
disappear after this age, and foreign languages have to be taught and learned
through a conscious and labored effort." (Lenneberg, 1967: 176).
Although he recognised that 'most individuals of average intelligence are able to
Ieam a second language after the beginning Oftheirsecond decade', he argued
that:
"...the incidence of "language learning blocks" rapid у increases after puberty
and foreign accents cannot easily be over∞me." (Lenneberg, 1967: 176).
In other words, while language acquisition outside the critical period might not
be entirely impossible, it would be qualitatively different, less natural and less
63
More intriguing information
1. Tourism in Rural Areas and Regional Development Planning2. Transfer from primary school to secondary school
3. 101 Proposals to reform the Stability and Growth Pact. Why so many? A Survey
4. The name is absent
5. The name is absent
6. Job quality and labour market performance
7. CREDIT SCORING, LOAN PRICING, AND FARM BUSINESS PERFORMANCE
8. On the Real Exchange Rate Effects of Higher Electricity Prices in South Africa
9. The name is absent
10. The name is absent