On Evolution of God-Seeking Mind



Conrad Montell

Rather, the focus is on imagination as a possible
adaptive response for early
Homo sapiens to that car-
dinal human fear: mortality (L
eyhausen 1973), and
on the associated memory devices essential for stor-
ing the products of imagination (L
anger 1982,
1972). Apprehension of death developed as a free
fear: the sensing of a danger that cannot be avoided
or fled from (L
eyhausen 1973; Langs 1996). Having
such apprehension, “we die a thousand deaths, that
is the price we pay for living a thousand lives”
(B
ronowski 1977, p25). When and to what extent
this apprehension became
conscious (in the ordinary
murky sense of the word) are questions beyond the
scope of this paper. This apprehension might have
developed as a consequence of that prereflective
consciousness S
artre and others consider as aware-
ness of an object and awareness that
it is not that
object (M
alhotra 1997). I avoid modern issues of
authenticity of self and self awareness: issues of
whether and to what extent such self and awareness
exist and are known by the individual, apart from
social content (W
eigert 1988). It seems that at least
some amount of self-awareness is required to enter
the state of being a self (M
artin 1985, p3). I intend
awareness: of self and other, and of mortality, to
mean some “knowing” of these things that leads to
behavior, whether or not the knowing can be
squeezed into thought and expressed. Thus, this
sense of
awareness encompasses various forms of
knowing, some of which were (and still are) ineffable:
anxiety, feelings of foreboding, dread, and individ-
ual moods that find expression in some form of hu-
man behavior, including inaction (out of fear) in a
situation calling for action. There is F
reud’s contro-
versial conception of a
death instinct to consider, as
well as other instinctual knowing that exists at the
borderline of animal and human awareness (B
rown
1959). Considering these levels of the knowing of
fear, I focus on that cardinal fear and on the potential
loss of vitality that I suggest paralleled its develop-
ment: “a number of factors, psychological as well as
physiological in nature, at work in causing actual,
concrete fears; the cardinal source (not the experi-
enced but the essential one) of the phenomenon of
fear as a whole, however, is man’s mortality” (L
ey-
hausen
1973, p248).

I argue that human imagination evolved as a way
of coping with that cardinal fear and its potentially
debilitating consequences. This fear could not be al-
leviated by further evolution of the external senses.
An inner sense offered an escape from a “predator”
that did not appear within the physical environ-
ment. This escape mechanism quite likely developed
as a distortion of sensory experience (L
orenz 1977;
L
eyhausen 1973). This disorder had survival value for
our species. L
orenz offers a clue in understanding
such a development: “Far from hindering the inves-
tigation of the organism affected by it, a pathological
disorder very often gives us the key to the under-
standing of how the organism works” (1977, p5).
This “key”, I suggest, is useful in understanding the
evolution of imagination as an adaptation. Human
self-awareness leading to an awareness of mortality
can be considered a disorder, and just that kind of
disorder that “gives us the key to understanding”
how that anomalous part of the human animal came
into being. “We have developed ‘organs’ only for
those aspects of reality of which, in the interest of
survival, it was imperative for our species to take ac-
count, so that selection pressure produced this par-
ticular cognitive apparatus” (L
orenz 1977, p7). Since
human imagination appears to be unique, it seems
reasonable to inquire as to what unique survival
problems might have developed for proto-humans.
I suggest that an evolving awareness of self and of
death of self led to a new kind of survival problem
that, in turn, led to the evolution of a new kind of
“solution”. As it evolved, imagination would lead to
the development of “belief”, a pro-attitude superim-
posed on information and experience, and to a new
kind of behavior: religious behavior. “Its original
function may have been to keep men’s minds in bal-
ance with the rest of nature, but what has led to its
own elaboration is a purpose it soon acquired: the
denial or masking of death” (L
anger 1982, p137).

“Religious behavior” is used here in a broad sense,
to include the nascent mental activity hominids,
newly aware of self and mortality, might have en-
gaged in individually and, as emerging language
made possible, in small groups. With regard to inves-
tigating the sources of religious behavior, there is, of
course, a great rift in human views of “mind”, “soul”,
and individual afterlife: a largely unspoken-of di-
chotomy between scientists and secular academi-
cians on the one hand, and the rest of the world on
the other, between the staunch materialists (mo-
nists) and the mass of people (dualists) who feel that
mind and soul exist as non-material stuff. “I suppose
most people in our civilization accept some kind of
dualism. They think they have both a mind and a
body. But that is emphatically not the current view
among the professionals in philosophy, artificial in-
telligence, neurobiology, and cognitive science”
(S
earle 1997, p43). Few professionals address this
rift. There is some risk in doing so, especially when
the different views are taken beyond academia. Our

Evolution and Cognition 3 2002, Vol. 8, No. 1



More intriguing information

1. The name is absent
2. DISCRIMINATORY APPROACH TO AUDITORY STIMULI IN GUINEA FOWL (NUMIDA MELEAGRIS) AFTER HYPERSTRIATAL∕HIPPOCAMP- AL BRAIN DAMAGE
3. Problems of operationalizing the concept of a cost-of-living index
4. Towards Learning Affective Body Gesture
5. The name is absent
6. Assessing Economic Complexity with Input-Output Based Measures
7. INSTITUTIONS AND PRICE TRANSMISSION IN THE VIETNAMESE HOG MARKET
8. Reconsidering the value of pupil attitudes to studying post-16: a caution for Paul Croll
9. The name is absent
10. The name is absent
11. Migration and Technological Change in Rural Households: Complements or Substitutes?
12. Death as a Fateful Moment? The Reflexive Individual and Scottish Funeral Practices
13. Macro-regional evaluation of the Structural Funds using the HERMIN modelling framework
14. The name is absent
15. Experience, Innovation and Productivity - Empirical Evidence from Italy's Slowdown
16. Gender and aquaculture: sharing the benefits equitably
17. The name is absent
18. The name is absent
19. SOME ISSUES CONCERNING SPECIFICATION AND INTERPRETATION OF OUTDOOR RECREATION DEMAND MODELS
20. The Dynamic Cost of the Draft