Asymmetric transfer of the dynamic motion aftereffect between first- and second-order cues and among different second-order cues



Journal of Vision (2007) 7(8):1, 1-12

Schofield, Ledgeway, & Hutchinson

11


References

Adelson, E. H., & Bergen, J. R. (1985). Spatiotemporal
energy models for the perception of motion.
Journal
of the Optical Society of America A, Optics and image
science, 2, 284-299. [PubMed]

Adelson, E. H., & Bergen, J. R. (1986). The extraction of
spatio-temporal energy in human and machine vision.
In
Proceedings of the workshop on motion: Repre-
sentation and analysis
(pp. 151- 156). Charleston,
SC: IEEE Computer Society.

Ashida, H., & Osaka, N. (1994). Difference of spatial
frequency selectivity between static and flicker motion
aftereffects.
Perception, 23, 1313-1320. [PubMed]

Baker, C. L., Jr. (1999). Central neural mechanisms for
detecting second-order motion.
Current Opinion in
Neurobiology, 9,
461- 466. [PubMed]

Baker, C. L., Jr., Mortin, C. L., Prins, N., Kingdom, F.
A., & Dumoulin, S. O. (2006). Visual cortex
responses to different texture-defined boundaries:
An fMRI study [
Abstract]. Journal of Vision, 6
(6):209, 209, http://journalofvision.org/6/6/209/,
doi:10.1167/6.6.209.

Benton, C. P. (2002). Gradient-based analysis of non-
Fourier motion.
Vision Research, 42, 2869-2877.
[
PubMed]

Benton, C. P. (2004). A role for contrast-normalisation in
second-order motion perception.
Vision Research, 44,
91-98. [PubMed]

Benton, C. P., & Johnston, A. (1997). First-order motion
from contrast modulated noise?
Vision Research, 37,
3073-3078. [PubMed]

Benton, C. P., & Johnston, A. (2001). A new approach to
analysing texture-defined motion.
Proceedings of the
Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences,
268,
2435-2443. [PubMed] [Article]

Benton, C. P., Johnston, A., McOwan, P. W., & Victor, J. D.
(2001). Computational modeling of non-Fourier
motion: Further evidence for a single luminance-based
mechanism.
Journal of the Optical Society of America
A, Optics, image science, and vision, 18,
2204-2208.
[
PubMed]

Bex, P. J., Verstraten, F. A., & Mareschal, I. (1996).
Temporal and spatial frequency tuning of the flicker
motion aftereffect.
Vision Research, 36, 2721-2727.
[
PubMed]

Blakemore, C., & Campbell, F. W. (1969). On the
existence of neurones in the human visual system
selectively sensitive to the orientation and size of retinal
images.
The Journal of Physiology, 203, 237-260.
[
PubMed] [Article]

Bruce, V., Green, P. R., & Georgeson, M. A. (1996).
Visual perception: Physiology, psychology and ecology
(3rd ed.). Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Cavanagh, P., & Mather, G. (1989). Motion: The long and
short of it.
Spatial Vision, 4, 103-129. [PubMed]

Chubb, C., & Sperling, G. (1988). Drift-balanced random
stimuli: A general basis for studying non-Fourier
motion perception.
Journal of the Optical Society of
America A, Optics and image science, 5,
1986 -2007.
[
PubMed]

Cruickshank, A. G. (2006). Supra-threshold interactions
between second-order cues: Modulations in contrast,
orientation and binocular disparity
. Unpublished
doctoral dissertation, University of Birmingham, UK.

Cruickshank, A. G., & Schofield, A. J. (2005). Transfer of
tilt after-effects between second-order cues.
Spatial
Vision, 18,
379-397. [PubMed]

Culham, J. C., Nishida, S., Ledgeway, T., Cavanagh, P.,
von Grunau, M. W., Kwas, M., et al. (1998). Higher
order effects. In G. Mather, F. Verstraten, & S. Anstis
(Eds.),
The motion after-effect: A modern perspective
(pp. 85 -124). Cambridge: MIT Press.

Derrington, A. M., & Badcock, D. R. (1985). Separate
detectors for simple and complex grating patterns?
Vision Research, 25, 1869-1878. [PubMed]

Fleet, D. J., & Langley, K. (1994). Computational
analysis of non-Fourier motion.
Vision Research,
34,
3057-3079. [PubMed]

Georgeson, M. A., & Schofield, A. J. (2002). Shading
and texture: Separate information channels with a
common adaptation mechanism?
Spatial Vision, 16,
59-76. [PubMed]

Gibson, J. J., & Radner, M. (1937). Adaptation, after-
effect and contrast in the perception of tilted lines: I.
Quantitative studies.
Journal of Experimental Psy-
chology, 20,
453-467.

Gray, R., & Regan, D. (1998). Spatial frequency discrim-
ination and detection characteristics for gratings
defined by orientation texture.
Vision Research, 38,
2601-2617. [PubMed]

von Grunau, M. (1986). A motion aftereffect for long-
range stroboscopic apparent motion.
Perception &
Psychophysics, 40,
31-38. [PubMed]

Gurnsey, R., Fleet, D., & Potechin, C. (1998). Second-
order motions contribute to vection.
Vision Research,
38,
2801-2816. [PubMed]

Hutchinson, C. V., & Ledgeway, T. (2006). Sensitivity to
spatial and temporal modulations of first-order and
second-order motion.
Vision Research, 46, 324-335.
[
PubMed]



More intriguing information

1. Stakeholder Activism, Managerial Entrenchment, and the Congruence of Interests between Shareholders and Stakeholders
2. Cultural Neuroeconomics of Intertemporal Choice
3. The name is absent
4. The name is absent
5. Thresholds for Employment and Unemployment - a Spatial Analysis of German Regional Labour Markets 1992-2000
6. Public Debt Management in Brazil
7. Performance - Complexity Comparison of Receivers for a LTE MIMO–OFDM System
8. Olfactory Neuroblastoma: Diagnostic Difficulty
9. The voluntary welfare associations in Germany: An overview
10. Modellgestützte Politikberatung im Naturschutz: Zur „optimalen“ Flächennutzung in der Agrarlandschaft des Biosphärenreservates „Mittlere Elbe“