Target Acquisition in Multiscale Electronic Worlds



19

pointing time should become independent of view size (Equation 7). All three experiments
used the same basic experimental protocol, which we first describe.

5.1. Methods

5.1.1. The Document

We had to circumvent a common problem of zooming interfaces, known as desert fog: one
gets lost if one zooms-in an empty region of a multiscale document (Jul & Furnas, 1998).
Since we were interested in the basic sensory-motor processes involved in target reaching,
rather than the higher cognitive processes of orientation, we used a document designed to
preclude disorientation. We used an electronic map containing nothing but an infinite set of
equidistant concentric circles centered around the target. Zooming-in caused a radial
expansion, and zooming-out a radial shrinking of the current document selection around the
screen center, but the number of visible arcs displayed in the document was held roughly
constant due to a self-similar fractal generation mechanism. When the target was out of sight
(as in Figure 17), our participants always knew in what direction and how far they had to
move their view, since target direction was specified by the perpendiculars to the tangents to
the visible arcs and target distance by arc curvature, the inverse of the radius
D.

Figure 17. A schematic illustration of the array of concentric circles with the target located out of sight.
The view, shown as an empty rectangle, intercepts four arcs which specify target direction and distance.
The radius and the arrow, not shown in the experimental display, appear here for explanatory reasons.

We had recourse to semantic zooming (Perlin & Fox, 1993; Furnas & Bederson, 1995). As
long as the magnification was insufficient to allow target representation, a constant-size (i.e.,
zoom-insensitive) beacon represented the target. As soon as, thanks to zooming-in, the target
could be displayed with more than one screen pixel, it replaced the beacon, the document then
revealing a colored disc that was zoomable and could be clicked.

5.1.2. Input device

Zooming was controlled with the left, non-preferred hand by rotating a game-controller
throttle. Panning was controlled with the right, preferred hand by operating a mouse (in
Experiment 1) or a Wacom stylus set to the relative mode (in Experiment 2). For both
zooming and panning, the control was in zero-order mode (i.e., angles were directly converted
into scale levels, and positions into positions, respectively).

There was no cursor. A stationary crosshair permanently shown at the center of the view
served for aiming, as in shooting games. Moving the panning device with the right hand



More intriguing information

1. Non Linear Contracting and Endogenous Buyer Power between Manufacturers and Retailers: Empirical Evidence on Food Retailing in France
2. Initial Public Offerings and Venture Capital in Germany
3. Gender stereotyping and wage discrimination among Italian graduates
4. The name is absent
5. The name is absent
6. The storage and use of newborn babies’ blood spot cards: a public consultation
7. The name is absent
8. The bank lending channel of monetary policy: identification and estimation using Portuguese micro bank data
9. The name is absent
10. The name is absent
11. Equity Markets and Economic Development: What Do We Know
12. Internationalization of Universities as Internationalization of Bildung
13. CAPACITAÇÃO GERENCIAL DE AGRICULTORES FAMILIARES: UMA PROPOSTA METODOLÓGICA DE EXTENSÃO RURAL
14. The name is absent
15. Keynesian Dynamics and the Wage-Price Spiral:Estimating a Baseline Disequilibrium Approach
16. Une Gestion des ressources humaines à l'interface des organisations : vers une GRH territoriale ?
17. The name is absent
18. The name is absent
19. FISCAL CONSOLIDATION AND DECENTRALISATION: A TALE OF TWO TIERS
20. Road pricing and (re)location decisions households